Phonology - Segmental |
Pre-/post-nasalized stops |
Analysis posits that the stop is the most relevant underlying phoneme. Comment in notes on whether the nasal contour is understood as a phonetic (allophonic) effect, or is phonologically contrastive. |
no |
|
Frank 1990:10 |
|
|
|
Phonology - Segmental |
Glottalized/ejective consonants |
Phonemic contrast [NOT counting glottal stop/fricative] |
no |
|
Frank 1990:10 |
|
|
|
Phonology - Segmental |
Palatalized stops |
Phonemic contrast |
no |
|
Frank 1990:10 |
|
|
|
Phonology - Segmental |
Phonemic vowel length |
Does the language have long and short vowels? |
no |
|
Frank 1990:11 |
|
|
|
Phonology - Segmental |
Phonemic glottalization/laryngealization of vowels |
|
no |
|
Frank 1990:11 |
|
|
|
Phonology - Segmental |
Complex onsets |
Onset consists of more than one consonant phoneme |
yes |
A (voiced or voiceless) stop plus r |
Frank 1990:11 |
|
|
|
Phonology - Segmental |
No codas |
*(C)VC [no also equals highly constrained] |
no |
|
Frank 1990:11 |
|
|
|
Phonology - Segmental |
Word-final coda required |
Do all syllables end in a consonant? |
no |
|
Frank 1990:11 |
|
|
|
Phonology - Suprasegmental |
Contrastive tones |
Note how many contrastive tones |
no |
more likely not |
Frank 1990:10-18 |
|
|
|
Phonology - Suprasegmental |
Contrastive stress |
Does stress occur on different syllables with meaning difference? |
no |
|
Frank 1985:14 |
|
|
|
Phonology - Suprasegmental |
Nasalization property of morpheme or syllable |
In contrast to nasalization as a property of segments |
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Phonology - Suprasegmental |
Nasal spreading across some morpheme boundaries |
Do some affixes or other morphemes take the nasal/oral properties of the root they attach to? |
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Phonology - Suprasegmental |
Vowel harmony |
|
no |
|
Frank 1985:17-9 |
|
|
|
Morphology - General |
Verbal fusion (2+ categories marked by portmanteau morphemes on verb) |
Verb combines two or more categories (tense, aspect, mood, person, number, etc.) in portmanteau morphemes{ [ignore proclitics unless they are fused with values other than person/number] |
no |
|
Frank 1985:60-104 |
|
|
|
Morphology - General |
Inflection manifested by replacement of segmental or suprasegmental phonemes |
Stem change, tone |
no |
|
Frank 1985:39-104 |
|
|
|
Morphology - General |
Verbal synthesis (1+ inflectional categories marked by verbal affixes) |
Morphological complexity in verbs - multiple inflectional affixes in a single verb word |
yes |
|
Frank 1985:60-87 |
|
|
|
Morphology - General |
Prefixing/suffixing inflectional morph: strongly prefixing |
There are many more prefixes than suffixes |
no |
|
Frank 1990:4 |
|
|
|
Morphology - General |
Prefixing/suffixing inflectional morph: strongly suffixing |
There are many more suffixes than prefixes |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:4 |
|
|
|
Morphology - General |
Prefixing/suffixing inflectional morph: roughly equal or one weakly preferred |
The numbers of suffixes and prefixes are not notably different |
no |
|
Frank 1990:4 |
|
|
|
Morphology - General |
Reduplication: full |
The full morpheme is reduplicated |
yes |
When an adjective is repeated it expresses a greater degree of the quality of it |
Frank 1985:43 |
|
|
|
Morphology - General |
Reduplication: partial |
Only part of the morpheme is reduplicated |
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Morphology - Compounding, auxiliaries, light verbs |
Productive NN compounding |
Noun compounds created from two noun phrases are common and systematically produced |
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Morphology - Compounding, auxiliaries, light verbs |
Productive VV serialization (without compounding) |
Verb roots can be combined in a single predicate without markers of subordination (distinct from subordinating construction) or distinct inflection |
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Morphology - Compounding, auxiliaries, light verbs |
Productive VV compounding |
Serial verb constructions involve chaining of roots together in one morphophonological word |
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Morphology - Compounding, auxiliaries, light verbs |
Verb-adjunct (aka light verb) constructions |
There is a set of semantically weak verbs used in complex verbal constructions, e.g. 'take a nap' |
no |
|
Frank 1990:46 |
|
|
|
Morphology - Compounding, auxiliaries, light verbs |
Auxiliary verb(s) |
There are verbs that accompany main verbs of clauses and take grammatical marking not expressed by main verbs |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:46 |
|
|
|
Morphology - Incorporation |
Incorporation of nouns into verbs is a productive intransitivizing process |
Verb contains nominal segment |
no |
However, it seems that some nouns have been incorporated into verbs of "putting" (Frank 1989:24-25 |
Frank 1985:60-104 |
|
|
|
Morphology - Incorporation |
Productive incorporation of other elements (adjectives, locatives, etc.) into verbs |
Like noun incorporation, but incorporated elements are not nouns |
no |
|
Frank 1985:60-104 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Gender and noun classification |
Noun classes/genders |
Nouns are organized into sets with distinct morphological treatment; usually affects all nouns and involves agreement within the NP |
yes |
The shape of the noun refered may have some morphosyntactic correlates as to the verb or normimal predicate it appears with |
Frank 1989:22 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Gender and noun classification |
Number of noun classes/genders |
Note the (approximate) total number of noun classes/genders |
6 |
long objects, flat objects, three-dimensional objects, liquids, containers, and objects with specialized holders |
Frank 1989:22 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Gender and noun classification |
Noun classifiers (distinct from noun classes/genders) |
Nouns are organized into sets, but only a limited set of nouns may be implicated, with no or limited agreement marking. If only numeral classifiers exist, indicate yes but explain. |
no |
|
Frank 1989:39-59 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Gender and noun classification |
Sex is a relevant category in noun class(ification) system for animates |
Masculine, feminine, neuter |
no |
|
Frank 1989:22 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Gender and noun classification |
Sex is a relevant category in noun class(ification) system for inanimates |
|
no |
|
Frank 1989:22 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Gender and noun classification |
Animacy (w/o reference to sex) is a relevant category in the noun class(ification) system |
Animate/inanimate, human/non-human |
no |
|
Frank 1989:22 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Gender and noun classification |
Sex/gender distinction only in 3rd person pronouns |
add in notes section whether gender is present in other PNs or not in any PNs; consider with reference to pronouns and person marking only |
no |
|
Frank 1985:34 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Gender and noun classification |
Shape is a relevant category in the noun class(ification) system for animates |
|
no |
|
Frank 1989:22 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Gender and noun classification |
Shape is a relevant category in the noun class(ification) system for inanimates |
|
yes |
|
Frank 1989:22 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Gender and noun classification |
"Repeater" classifiers |
Where no distinct classifier exists, a copy of the noun itself may function in the morphosyntactic classifier "slot" |
no |
|
Frank 1989:39-59 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Gender and noun classification |
Numeral classifiers (specific to numerals) |
Special classifier forms that occur only with numerals |
no |
|
Frank 1989:39-59 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Gender and noun classification |
Classifiers used as derivational suffixes to derive nouns |
Verb + classifier = 'thing for doing V, thing that does V, etc.' |
no |
|
Frank 1989:39-59 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Number |
Singular number may be marked on the noun |
Often occurs in a small subset of nouns if a single entity is referred to, e.g. insects that normally occur in groups |
no |
|
Frank 1989:39-59 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Number |
Plural affix on noun |
|
no |
|
Frank 1990:29 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Number |
Plural marked by stem change or tone on noun |
|
no |
|
Frank 1990:29 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Number |
Plural marked by reduplication of noun |
|
no |
|
Frank 1990:29 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Number |
Plural word/clitic |
|
no |
|
Frank 1990:29 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Number |
Plural marked on human or animate nouns only |
|
no |
|
Frank 1990:29 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Number |
Pronominal plural: stem + nominal plural affix |
Pronouns use a nominal plural affix not specific to pronouns |
no |
|
Frank 1990:26 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Number |
Unique associative plural marker |
e.g. 'John and his associates', 'John and them' |
no |
more likely noy |
|
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Definiteness and clusivity |
Definite or specific articles |
Definite = particular referent known to both speaker and addressee; specific = particular referent known to speaker only |
no |
the demonstrative eima "this one" is used as a definiteness marker |
Frank 1990:33 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Definiteness and clusivity |
Marker of definiteness distinct from demonstratives |
Focus on articles/markers whose primary function is to mark definiteness |
no |
|
Frank 1990:33 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Definiteness and clusivity |
Indefinite or non-specific article |
or marker |
no |
The cardinal numeral inʔgui "one" is used as a marked or inefiniteness |
Frank 1990:33 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Definiteness and clusivity |
Inclusive/exclusive: in free pronominals |
Inclusive =us + you, exclusive = us but not you |
yes |
However, the inclusive form seems to be rare. The exclusive form is the most frequent. |
Frank 1990:26 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Definiteness and clusivity |
Inclusive/exclusive: in verbal inflection (bound) |
|
no |
|
Frank 1989; Frank ? |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Definiteness and clusivity |
Distance contrasts in demonstratives (number) |
Note the number of distances in the demonstrative system |
2 |
|
Frank 1985:35 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Definiteness and clusivity |
Other contrasts in demonstratives (visibility, elevation, etc.) |
|
yes |
Pronouns are distinguished between deictic and anaphoric |
Frank 1985:35 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Pronominal categories |
Gender in 3sg pronouns |
|
no |
|
Frank 1985:34 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Pronominal categories |
Gender in 3pl pronouns |
|
no |
|
Frank 1985:34 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Pronominal categories |
Gender in 1st and/or 2nd person pronouns |
|
no |
|
Frank 1985:34 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Pronominal categories |
Formal/informal distinction in pronouns |
Polite pronominal variants or differential avoidance of pronouns |
no |
|
Frank 1985:34 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Pronominal categories |
Reflexive pronouns |
e.g. English 'himself', Spanish 'se'; distinct form(s) from basic (non-reflexive) pronominals; distinct from reflexive verbal affix |
no |
However, the adjective kingui "same" is used after the subject to indicate reflexivity. This can be used with subject pronouns too. |
Frank ?:25 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Case and adpositions |
Adpositions mark core NPs |
Prepositions or postpositions mark subjects, objects, beneficiaries/recipients |
yes |
|
Frank 1985:46 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Case and adpositions |
Case: number of cases |
Note the number of grammatical relations that may be morphologically marked on the noun |
7 |
|
Frank 1985:46 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Case and adpositions |
Case: only non-core arguments morphologically marked |
Subjects, objects, beneficiaries/recipients NOT marked, but other grammatical relations are |
no |
|
Frank 1985:46 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Case and adpositions |
Case: symmetrical |
All NPs marked if in appropriate syntactic relation; no distinction in marking based on semantics (type of entity) |
no |
|
Frank 1985:49 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Case and adpositions |
Case: asymmetrical |
Semantically defined subset of NPs marked for case, e.g. animates |
yes |
Ergative case is dependent on the pragamtic status of the subject with respect to the object. The subject gets ergative case if it is less "given" than the object |
Frank 1985:49 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Case and adpositions |
Case: suffix or postpositional clitic |
|
yes |
postposition |
Frank 1985:46 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Case and adpositions |
Case: prefix or prepositional clitic |
|
no |
|
Frank 1985:46 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Case and adpositions |
Case: infix or inpositional clitic |
|
no |
|
Frank 1985:46 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Case and adpositions |
Case: stem change |
|
no |
|
Frank 1985:46 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Case and adpositions |
Case: tone |
|
no |
|
Frank 1985:46 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Case and adpositions |
Case: comitative = instrumental |
Same marking for 'with a person' and 'with an instrument' |
yes |
|
Frank 1985:50 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Numerals |
Base-2 |
At least some part of the system involves base-2 |
no |
|
Frank 1990:27 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Numerals |
Base-5 |
At least some part of the system involves base-5 |
no |
|
Frank 1990:27 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Numerals |
Base-10 |
At least some part of the system involves base-10 |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:27 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Numerals |
Other base (specify) |
4, 20, etc. |
no |
|
Frank 1990:27 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Numerals |
Etymological transparency in any numerals under 5 |
e.g. two = 'eye-quantity' |
no |
But based on the list of 1-10 numbers, it seems that there is not etymological transparency. |
|
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Numerals |
Numerals do not go above 5 |
'Many' or some other non-exact term used |
no |
|
Frank 1985:36 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Numerals |
Numerals do not go above 10 |
'Many' or some other non-exact term used |
no |
|
Frank 1985:36 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Other nominal |
Tense or aspect inflection on non-verbal predicates |
i.e. nominal or adjectival |
no |
|
Frank 1985:25-6, 46-59 |
|
|
|
Nominal Categories - Other nominal |
Person inflection on non-verbal predicates |
i.e. nominal or adjectival |
no |
|
Frank 1985:25-6, 46-59 |
|
|
|
Nominal Syntax - Possession |
Pronominal possessive affixes: prefix on N |
alienable/inalienable? |
yes |
These pronominal prefixes are the same as the object markers on the verb. These possessive are only used with kin terms |
Frank 1989:22 |
|
|
|
Nominal Syntax - Possession |
Pronominal possessive affixes: suffix on N |
alienable/inalienable? |
no |
|
Frank 1989:22 |
|
|
|
Nominal Syntax - Possession |
Head/dependent marking in possessive NP: dependent |
e.g. 'the boy-'s dog' |
yes |
The possessive marker is the postposition /zei/ |
Frank 1985:55 |
|
|
|
Nominal Syntax - Possession |
Head/dependent marking in possessive NP: head |
e.g. 'the boy his-dog' |
no |
|
Frank 1985:55 |
|
|
|
Nominal Syntax - Possession |
Possessive classifiers |
There are special classifiers that occur with possessed entities |
no |
|
Frank 1985:39-59 |
|
|
|
Nominal Syntax - Possession - Alienability |
Morphological marking of inalienable possession |
Where inalienable possession differs from alienable, the former takes a morphological marker (may include an associated free particle/pronoun) |
no |
|
Frank 1985:55-7 |
|
|
|
Nominal Syntax - Possession - Alienability |
Morphological marking of alienable possession |
Where inalienable possession differs from alienable, the latter takes a morphological marker (may include an associated free particle/pronoun) |
no |
|
Frank 1985:55-7 |
|
|
|
Nominal Syntax - Possession - Alienability |
Default marker for inalienably possessed nouns if unpossessed |
An inalienable noun that is in an unpossessed state must have a derivational affix or associated form |
no |
|
Frank 1985:55-7 |
|
|
|
Nominal Syntax - Possession - Alienability |
Inalienable possession of kin terms |
'my-father' but *father |
yes |
This is marked by person prefixes on the noun. However, when the noun is used as vocative no personal preffix is added. |
Frank 1990:19 |
|
|
|
Nominal Syntax - Possession - Alienability |
Inalienable possession of body parts (human/animal) |
'my-leg' but *leg |
no |
|
Frank 1985:55-7 |
|
|
|
Nominal Syntax - Possession - Alienability |
Generic human nouns are obligatorily bound/possessed |
Human nouns must co-occur with another noun (e.g. Hup-man, NonIndian-woman, but *man) |
no |
|
Frank 1985:55-7 |
|
|
|
Nominal Syntax - Adjectives |
Underived adjectives |
There are underived adjectives which do not have counterparts in other word classes |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:19-20 |
|
|
|
Nominal Syntax - Adjectives |
Gender inflection on adjectives within the NP |
There is gender agreement/concord (animate/inanimate or masc/fem, etc.) within the NP, e.g. la casa blanca, el perro blanco |
no |
Based on the examples |
Frank 1990:19-20 |
|
|
|
Nominal Syntax - Derivation |
Productive nominalizing morphology: action/state (arrive/arrival) |
There is a morpheme which derives an event from a verb |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:19 |
|
|
|
Nominal Syntax - Derivation |
Productive nominalizing morphology: agentive (sing/singer) |
There is a morpheme which derives an agent or subject from a verb |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:19 |
|
|
|
Nominal Syntax - Derivation |
Productive nominalizing morphology: object (sing/song) |
There is a morpheme which derives a patient or object from a verb |
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Nominal Syntax - Derivation |
Productive verbalizing morphology |
There is a morpheme which derives a verb from a noun or adjective |
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Nominal Syntax - Other |
NP coordination and comitative phrases marked differently |
'John and Mary went to market' is marked differently from 'John went to market with Mary' |
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Aspect and tense |
Dedicated past marker(s) |
Past tense is regularly morphologically marked on the verb or elsewhere |
no |
Aspect is marked on the verb rather than tense.Ika distinguishes perfective and imperfective aspect. When the verb occurs in its bare form in a sentence, it expresses past tense |
Frank 1990:56-58 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Aspect and tense |
Multiple past tenses, distinguishing distance from time of reference |
e.g. distant vs. recent past |
no |
|
Frank 1990:56-58 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Aspect and tense |
Multiple future tenses, distinguishing distance from time of reference |
e.g. imminent vs. distant future |
yes |
There are at least two future tense markers: /-ngua/ for future simple and /-iwa/ for inmediate future |
Frank 1990:61 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Aspect and tense |
Dedicated future or non-past marker(s) |
|
yes |
|
Frank 1990:61 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Aspect and tense |
Tense-aspect affixes: prefix |
|
no |
|
Frank 1990:56-58 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Aspect and tense |
Tense-aspect affixes: suffix |
|
yes |
|
Frank 1990:56-58 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Aspect and tense |
Tense-aspect affixes: tone or ablaut |
|
no |
|
Frank 1990:56-58 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Aspect and tense |
Tense-aspect suppletion |
|
no |
|
|
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Mood |
Dedicated imperative morpheme or verb form |
There is a special morpheme (or morphemes, or a bare verb root where inflection is normally expected) used to signal imperative (command) mood |
no |
A verbal periphrastic strategy is used where the verb occurs uninflected and it is followed by a stressed auxiliary verb |
Frank 1990:86-7 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Mood |
Polite imperative morpheme |
There is a distinct morpheme for polite imperative constructions (specify if it has other functions in the language) |
no |
|
Frank 1985:117-120 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Mood |
Difference between negation in imperative (prohibitive) and declarative clauses |
There are different strategies for marking negation in imperative and declarative clauses |
no |
|
Frank 1985:118-122 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Mood |
Dedicated hortative morpheme or verb form (1pl or 3rd person imperative) |
as opposed to imperative; the person in control of desired state of affairs is not the addressee; ex: 'Let's sing' / 'Let him sing' |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:88 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Mood |
Situational possibility: affix on verb |
Inflectional marking of capacity to do something |
yes |
This is a verbal suufix |
Frank 1990:59-60 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Mood |
Situational possibility: verbal construction |
|
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Mood |
Situational possibility: other marking |
|
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Mood |
Epistemic possibility: affix on verb |
Modal expressing hypothesis |
no |
|
Frank 1985:60-104 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Mood |
Epistemic possibility: verbal construction |
|
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Mood |
Epistemic possibility: other marking |
|
no |
|
|
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Mood |
Marking of expected/unexpected action or result |
There is inflectional marking of expected/unexpected |
no |
|
Frank 1985:60-104 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Mood |
Verbal frustrative |
Modal expressing frustration ("in vain") |
no |
|
Frank 1985:60-104 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Mood |
Verbal habitual |
Modal expressing habituality |
no |
|
Frank 1985:60-104 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Mood |
Apprehensive construction |
There is a single morpheme or verb form to mean '(be careful lest) X happens' |
yes |
This is a prohibitive verbal suffix which is distinc from a negative imperative in which it is more than of an warning |
|
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Mood |
Reality status marking on verbs |
There are dedicated morpheme(s) for realis/irrealis 'actualized/unactualized events' |
no |
|
|
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Mood |
Affect markers (positive/negative) |
Note whether these inflectional markers are positive or negative |
no |
|
|
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Directionals |
Directional elements affixed to the verb |
There are grammaticalized elements indicating movement away, toward, there and back, etc. |
no |
|
Frank 1985:60-104 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Evidentiality |
Grammaticalized visual |
Indicates information has been witnessed visually - indicate only if an overt marker |
yes |
This is a verbal suffix or it can also be attached to the auxiliary verb |
Frank 1990:77 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Evidentiality |
Grammaticalized nonvisual |
Indicates information has been sensed firsthand but not visually (usually heard; also smelled, tasted, felt) |
no |
|
Frank 1985:60-104 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Evidentiality |
Grammaticalized inferential |
Indicates information has not been experienced firsthand, but inferred from some kind of evidence - indicate only if an overt marker. |
no |
|
Frank 1985:60-104 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Evidentiality |
Grammaticalized reportive |
Indicates speaker is not responsible for veracity of statement, merely reporting; 'allegedly' |
no |
|
Frank 1985:60-104 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Evidentiality |
Grammaticalized quotative |
Indicate presence of adjacent representation of repeated discourse |
yes |
This evidential verbal suffix expresses certainty |
Frank 1990:77 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Evidentiality |
Other evidential |
Any other evidential values not represented above |
yes |
|
Frank 1989:19 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Evidentiality |
Evidentiality: verb affix or clitic |
|
yes |
|
Frank 1990:77 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Evidentiality |
Evidentiality: part of tense system |
Includes portmanteau morphs |
no |
|
Frank 1985:60-104 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Evidentiality |
Evidentiality: separate particle |
|
no |
|
Frank 1985:60-104 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Evidentiality |
Evidentiality: modal morpheme |
|
no |
|
Frank 1985:60-104 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Verbal number |
Verbal number suppletion |
|
no |
|
Frank 1989:21 |
|
|
|
Verbal Categories - Other |
Social interaction markers |
Note the type of interaction |
no |
|
|
|
|
|
Word Order |
No fixed basic constituent order |
|
no |
|
Frank 1990:3 |
|
|
|
Word Order |
VS in intransitive clauses |
Verb precedes subject |
no |
|
|
|
|
|
Word Order |
VS in transitive clauses |
|
no |
|
Frank 1990:3 |
|
|
|
Word Order |
VO in transitive clauses |
Verb precedes object |
no |
|
Frank 1990:3 |
|
|
|
Word Order |
OS in transitive clauses |
Object precedes subject |
no |
|
Frank 1990:3 |
|
|
|
Word Order |
Preposition-Noun |
|
no |
|
Frank 1990:3 |
|
|
|
Word Order |
Noun-Postposition or case suffix |
|
yes |
|
Frank 1990:3 |
|
|
|
Word Order |
Gen-Noun |
Possessive phrase composed of a free possessor and its possessum has possessor first (e.g. John's book) |
yes |
This order is preffered and more frequent. |
Frank 1990:4 |
|
|
|
Word Order |
Noun-Gen |
Possessive phrase composed of a free possessor and its possessum has possessum first (e.g. 'book of John') |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:4 |
|
|
|
Word Order |
Adj-Noun |
Adjective precedes the noun |
no |
|
Frank 1990:3 |
|
|
|
Word Order |
Noun-Adj |
Adjective follows the noun |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:3 |
|
|
|
Word Order |
Dem-Noun |
|
yes |
|
Frank 1985:44 |
|
|
|
Word Order |
Noun-Dem |
|
no |
more likely not based on the examples |
Frank 1985:44 |
|
|
|
Word Order |
Num-Noun |
|
yes |
indefinite construction |
Frank 1985:41 |
|
|
|
Word Order |
Noun-Num |
|
yes |
definite construction |
Frank 1985:41 |
|
|
|
Word Order |
Noun-Rel |
Relative clause follows noun that it modifies |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:5 |
|
|
|
Word Order |
Rel-Noun |
Relative clause precedes noun that it modifies |
yes |
But this order is rare |
Frank 1990:5 |
|
|
|
Word Order |
Re<Noun>l (internally headed relative) |
e.g. 'the dog cat chased-NMZR got away' ('the cat that the dog chased got away') |
yes |
This seems to be the most frequent strategy for relativization |
Frank 1990:99-100 |
|
|
|
Word Order |
Relative clause is correlative or adjoined |
e.g. 'what is running, the dog chased that cat' |
no |
|
Frank 1985:130-3 |
|
|
|
Word Order |
Question word is clause initial |
'what', 'who', etc. come first in interrogative clause |
yes |
|
Frank 1985:112 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Alignment |
Alignment of case marking in full NPs: nominative-accusative w/ marked accusative |
Objects of transitive clauses ('P') have a unique marker, while subjects of transitive ('A') and intransitive ('S') clauses are unmarked or share a different marker from that occurring on objects |
no |
|
Frank 1985:49 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Alignment |
Alignment of case marking in full NPs: nominative-accusative w/ marked nominative |
Subjects of transitive and intransitive clauses share a marker, while objects of transitives are unmarked |
no |
|
Frank 1990:8-10 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Alignment |
Alignment of case marking in full NPs: ergative-absolutive |
Subjects of intransitive clauses and objects of transitives share a unique marker, while subjects of transitive clauses are unmarked or have a different marker |
yes |
But the marking of the A relation is optional and is restricted to contexts in which A appears contigous to the verb and not in its canonical position before the object. |
Frank 1990:9-10 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Alignment |
Alignment of case marking in full NPs: tripartite |
Intransitive subjects, transitive subjects, and transitive objects all receive distinct case markers |
no |
|
Frank 1985:39-59 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Alignment |
Alignment of case marking in full NPs: active-inactive |
Subjects of intransitive clauses are treated two different ways: like subjects of transitives if they are more agent-like (e.g. he jumped), and like objects of transitives if they are more patient-like (e.g. he fell asleep) |
no |
|
Frank 1985:39-59 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Alignment |
Alignment of case marking of pronouns: marked accusative |
|
no |
|
Frank 1985:39-59 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Alignment |
Alignment of case marking of pronouns: marked nominative |
|
no |
No. More likely similar to that of nouns. |
Frank 1985:39-59 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Alignment |
Alignment of case marking of pronouns: ergative-absolutive |
yes, no, mixed, other |
no info |
More likely yes |
Frank 1985:39-59 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Alignment |
Alignment of case marking of pronouns: tripartite |
|
no |
More likely not |
Frank 1985:39-59 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Alignment |
Alignment of case marking of pronouns: active-inactive |
|
no |
More likely not |
Frank 1985:39-59 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Alignment |
Alignment of verbal person-marking: nominative-accusative |
Same as above, for pronominal affixes/clitics on verbs |
yes |
This is the predominant pattern. |
Frank 1990:8 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Alignment |
Alignment of verbal person-marking: ergative-absolutive |
yes, no, mixed, other |
yes |
However, this is highly restricted. There are few intransitive roots (mainly "experiential") which take the same person pronoun as those used for objects in transitive verbs. |
Frank 1990:8-9 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Alignment |
Alignment of verbal person-marking: active-inactive |
|
yes |
But only a small set of intransitive verbs shows this pattern |
Frank 1985:11 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Alignment |
Alignment of verbal person-marking: hierarchical |
Marking of A and P depends on their relative ranking on a hierarchy (usually 1>2>3 or 2>1>3) |
no |
|
Frank 1985:9-10, 60-104 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Alignment |
Alignment of verbal person-marking: split |
More than one of the above systems is represented in person marking, depending on e.g. person (e.g. 1/2 vs. 3), tense-aspect value, main vs. subordinate clause type, etc. |
yes |
|
Frank 1985:11 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Pronouns and person marking |
Pronominal subjects: pronouns in subject position |
Pronominal subjects are free pronouns that occur in the same position as full NP subjects |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:25 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Pronouns and person marking |
Pronominal subjects: prefixes on verb |
Pronominal subjects are marked as verbal prefixes (free pronouns may be another option) |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:7-8 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Pronouns and person marking |
Pronominal subjects: suffixes on verb |
Pronominal subjects are marked as verbal suffixes (free pronouns may be another option) |
yes |
Not sure why some subject pronouns appear as prefixes and others as suffixes. This may be related to the particular caracteristics of each subject suffix individually. |
Frank 1990.7-8 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Pronouns and person marking |
Pronominal subjects: clitics on variable host |
Pronominal subjects are clitics that can attach to verbs, nominal constituents, etc. |
no |
|
Frank 1990:25 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Pronouns and person marking |
Pronominal subjects: pronouns in non-subject position |
Pronominal subjects are free pronouns but do not normally occur in the position expected for full NP subjects |
no |
|
Frank 1990:25 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Pronouns and person marking |
Person marking on intransitive verbs |
Intransitive verbs take person-marking clitics/affixes |
yes |
|
Frank 1989:20 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Pronouns and person marking |
Person marking (of agents) on transitive verbs |
Transitive verbs take subject (A) markers |
yes |
|
Frank 1989:20 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Pronouns and person marking |
Person-marking (of objects) on transitive verbs |
Transitive verbs take object (P) markers |
yes |
|
Frank 1989:20 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Pronouns and person marking |
3rd person zero in verbal person marking: subjects |
3rd person subjects are not overtly marked within the verbal person-marking system |
yes |
Except for third person plural which are optionally marked on the verb |
Frank 1989:20 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Pronouns and person marking |
3rd person zero in verbal person marking: objects |
3rd person objects are not overtly marked within the verbal person-marking system |
yes |
|
Frank 1989:20 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Pronouns and person marking |
Number can be marked separately from person on the verb |
Verbal person marking exists, but number is (or can) be marked separately |
no |
|
Frank 1989:21 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Pronouns and person marking |
Possessive affixes/clitics on nouns are same as verbal person markers |
Where nouns take possessive affixes, these are the same as the person-marking affixes |
yes |
The set of verbal object pronouns is used instead of the verbal subject pronouns |
Frank 1989:22 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Pronouns and person marking |
Gender distinguished in verbal person markers |
For any person, verbal person markers exhibit different forms depending on the gender (masc/fem, animate/inanimate, etc.) of the referent |
no |
|
Frank 1985:67-71 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Valence and voice |
Ditransitive constructions: indirect object |
In ditransitives (e.g. 'John gives a book to Bill'), the theme (book) is treated in the same way as are objects of transitives, while the recipient/beneficiary (Bill) is treated differently |
yes |
|
Frank 1989:20 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Valence and voice |
Ditransitive constructions: double object |
In ditransitives (e.g. 'John gives Bill a book'), both the theme (book) and the recipient/beneficiary (Bill) is treated in the same way as are objects of transitives |
no |
|
Frank 1989:20 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Valence and voice |
Ditransitive constructions: secondary object |
In ditransitives, the recipient/beneficiary is treated in the same way as are objects of transitives, while the theme (book) is treated differently |
yes |
But only when the beneficiary/gol NP is human which outranks the object in animacy |
Frank 1989:20 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Valence and voice - Decreasing |
Reciprocal: dedicated morpheme |
Verb becomes reciprocal through use of reciprocal morpheme associated with the verb (may be attached to the verb root). This morpheme is only used to mean reciprocal. |
yes |
This is the same verbal suffix used for the reciprocal |
Frank 1990:71 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Valence and voice - Decreasing |
Reflexive: dedicated morpheme |
Verb becomes reflexive through use of reflexive morpheme associated with the verb (may be attached to the verb root). This morpheme is used only to mean reflexive. |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:71 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Valence and voice - Decreasing |
Reciprocal/reflexive: same morpheme |
Verb becomes reciprocal or reflexive through use of a morpheme that means either reciprocal or reflexive which attaches to the root of the verb |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:71 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Valence and voice - Decreasing |
Passive |
Passive voice usually involves a change to the verb, while the object of the active voice verb is promoted to subject in the passive voice, and the former subject is deleted/demoted |
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Valence and voice - Decreasing |
Antipassive |
Like passive, but deletes or demotes the object of a transitive verb; usually found in ergative languages |
no |
|
|
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Valence and voice - Decreasing |
Other intransitivizing morphology |
There is/are some other mechanism(s) for reducing valency |
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Valence and voice - Increasing |
Applicative: benefactive |
Applicative adds a beneficiary/maleficiary object argument to the verb |
yes |
It also encodes malefactive meaning |
Frank ?:24 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Valence and voice - Increasing |
Applicative: other |
Applicative adds some other object argument to the verb |
yes |
This suffix allows the verb to take an extra argument which in many case is implicit but cannot appear cross-referenced on the verb. In other cases it adds the possessor of a possessive clause as a verbal argument |
Frank 1985:93-96 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Valence and voice - Increasing |
Causative: prefix |
Causative is morphological and is attached before the root of the verb |
no |
|
Frank 1990:66 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Valence and voice - Increasing |
Causative: suffix |
Causative is morphological and is attached after the root of the verb |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:66 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Valence and voice - Increasing |
Causative marked by circumfix, stem change, or tone |
Morphological causative other than simple prefix/suffix |
yes |
There´s verbal stem change as a strategy for forming causative. However, what the author refers here as causative may be described as jussive "let hm go in"m for instance. |
Frank 1990:66 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Valence and voice - Increasing |
Causative: serial verb or analytical construction |
Causative construction that involves periphrasis or serialization |
yes |
This construction involves the verb "to cause" guaʔsa |
Frank 1990:66 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Valence and voice - Increasing |
Causative: dedicated 'make do by proxy' |
Indicates that the causer does not directly cause the action of the verb to be realized, but does so by inducing someone else to carry out the action, e.g. 'John had the house painted.' |
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Valence and voice - Increasing |
Causative: dedicated sociative |
Indicates that causer participates in event |
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Valence and voice - Increasing |
Other transitivizing morphology (adds valence) |
There is/are some other mechanism(s) for increasing valency |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:68 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Negation |
Clausal negator is a preposed element |
Clausal negator is a preposed element |
no |
|
Frank 1990:6 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Negation |
Clausal negator is a postposed element |
Clausal negator is a postposed element |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:6 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Negation |
Negatives: affix |
Negatives: affix |
yes |
This is a verbal suffix |
Frank 1990:6 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Negation |
Negatives: particle |
Negatives: particle |
no |
|
Frank 1985:120-121 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Negation |
Negatives: auxiliary verb |
Negatives: auxiliary verb |
no |
|
Frank 1985:120-121 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Negation |
Negatives: double |
Standard (non-emphatic) negation typically requires two morphemes, e.g. French 'ne V pas' |
no |
|
Frank 1985:120-121 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Negation |
Distinct negative form for 'NP does not exist' |
|
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Negation |
Distinct negative expression 'I don't know' |
Lexical expression or highly idiomatic phrase |
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Interrogatives |
Polar questions: interrogative particle |
Yes/no questions distinguished from declaratives by interrogative particle |
yes |
This particle occurs sentence-finally |
Frank 1990:6 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Interrogatives |
Polar questions: verb morphology |
Yes/no questions distinguished from declaratives by interrogative verb morphology |
no |
|
Frank 1990:6 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Interrogatives |
Polar questions: word order |
Yes/no questions distinguished from declaratives by word order (esp. subject-verb inversion) |
no |
|
Frank 1990:6 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Interrogatives |
Polar questions: intonation only |
Yes/no questions distinguished from declaratives by intonation only |
no |
|
Frank 1990:6 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Interrogatives |
Content questions: word order differs from declaratives |
Content questions distinguished from declaratives by word order (esp. subject-verb inversion) as well as by presence of Q-word (who, what, etc.) |
no |
|
Frank 1990:6 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Predication |
Predicate adjectives: verbal |
Adjectives act like verbs in predicative position |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:619-20 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Predication |
Predicate adjectives: nominal |
Adjectives act like nouns in predicative position |
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Predication |
Zero copula for predicate nominals is possible |
Predicate nominals may occur without a copula (i.e. grammatical in some circumstances, if not all) |
no info |
|
|
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Predication |
Headless relative clauses |
Compare Eng 'the one that fell' (but in Eng 'one' could be considered a head) |
no info |
more likely not |
Frank 19885:130-3 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Predication |
Headless relative clauses are the dominant or only form of relative clause |
Relative clauses that form a constituent with a head noun (in a single noun phrase) are rare or nonexistent; some descriptions may refer to adjoined or correlative clauses. |
no info |
more likely not |
Frank 19885:130-3 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Predication |
Relative clause may occur with a noun classifier/class marker |
It may be unclear whether the classifier is the nominal head of the construction or is an agreement marker on the relative clause |
no |
|
Frank 19885:130-3 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Predication |
Relativizer is a verbal affix |
|
no |
There is no relativizer |
Frank 1985:130-3 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Predication |
Morphological relativizer is homophonous with nominalizer |
The same morpheme marks a relative clause and is a nominalizer on verbs (and/or other word classes) |
no |
|
Frank 1985:130-3 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Desiderative expressions |
Grammaticalized verbal desiderative |
Indicates that the subject desires to carry out the action denoted by the verb (distinct from verb 'want', but may be grammaticalized from it) |
no |
|
Frank 1985:60-104 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Other |
Clause chaining |
Clauses can be grouped such that only one bears most of the verb morphology, and the others are marked as to whether they share a subject with this reference clause. |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:91 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Other |
Morphologically marked switch-reference system |
There are special markers to indicate same vs. different subject when two clauses are combined |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:91-5 |
|
|
|
Simple Clauses - Other |
Morphologically marked distinction between simultaneous and sequential clauses |
Morphology (usually on verb) distinguishes between clauses denoting events that occur at the same time or in sequence |
yes |
|
Frank 1990:91-5 |
|
|
|